The Phenomenology of Synaesthesia

نویسنده

  • E. M. Hubbard
چکیده

This article supplements our earlier paper on synaesthesia published in JCS (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001a). We discuss the phenomenology of synaesthesia in greater detail, raise several new questions that have emerged from recent studies, and suggest some tentative answers to these questions. I: Robustness of Synaesthetic Effects It was Francis Galton (1880) who first reported the condition called synaesthesia. He noticed that a certain number of people in the general population, who are otherwise completely normal, seemed to have a certain peculiarity: they experience sensations in multiple modalities in response to stimulation of one modality. For example, musical notes might evoke distinct colours; F might be red and C blue. Or the printed number 5 always ‘looks’ green, whereas 2 looks red. Recent evidence suggest that synaesthesia is a genuine sensory phenomenon, not a high-level memory association (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001a,b). This raises several new questions regarding the robustness of the colours evoked by specific graphemes. Do physical changes in the number affect the perceived colour? And, to what extent are the grapheme–colour correspondences universal, i.e., seen in a majority of synaesthetes? (a) Does it matter whether the letters are upper or lower case? We usually find that upper and lower case letters evoke the same colours, although the lower case letters were usually less saturated, or were ‘shiny’ or ‘patchy’ compared to the upper case letter, perhaps because upper case is learnt earlier. There are exceptions to this. For example, in ‘Sarah’ most letters followed the rule, but E had completely different colours for upper and lower case (‘E’ was green, while ‘e’ was red). (b) Does the font of the number or letter affect the colours? Prototypical fonts like Times Roman or Arial normally give the most vivid colour for that letter, but on occasion a ‘weird’ font like Gothic actually evokes a stronger colour. We suggest that such fonts might serve as ‘hyper-normal’ stimuli Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, No. 8, 2003, pp. 49–57 Correspondence: Center Brain and Cognition, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109, USA. Email: [email protected] that evoke even larger responses from the grapheme neurons than a more ‘prototypical’ font might. This would be an example of the peak-shift effect, which causes seagull chicks actually to prefer pecking at a long stick with three stripes to pecking at a real beak (see Ramachandran & Hirstein, 1999). (c) What if the number is presented in the ‘wrong’ colour? This produces a slight delay in naming the colour; the induced colour delays the ability to report the real colour. This effect — Stroop interference — shows that the colour associations are automatic (Dixon et al., 2000; Mattingley et al., 2001), but it does not necessarily show that it is sensory or perceptual (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001a,b). We have noticed that it also often produces a strong visceral discomfort like ‘nails scratching a blackboard’. Perhaps the ‘hyperconnectivity’ genes cause an excess of connections between sensory and limbic structures like the amygdala, so that even a trifling discord produces a disproportionately large abhorrence (see Cytowic, 1989/2002; Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001a). We are testing this theory by monitoring galvanic skin response (GSR), which indexes emotional arousal. (d) Does a given number evoke the same colour across different synaesthetes? Synaesthetic colour associations remain stable in any given synaesthete, even when tested over intervals of up to one year (Baron-Cohen et al., 1993). But does the same grapheme tend to evoke the same colour across different synaesthetes? The answer is no. One synaesthete might see A as red, another might see it as green (Day, 2001). But the associations are not random either. There is a higher chance that A will be red than that it will be (say) blue or yellow. Although such trends have been noticed before no explanation was offered. We suggest that it may reflect the manner in which phonemes (in higher synaesthetes — see below) are mapped near the TPO junction in a systematic topographic manner, which in turn would make certain types of cross-activation more likely than others (e.g. front vowels might activate long wavelengths). Similarly, graphemes might be mapped in ‘form space’ in the fusiform in such a way that certain colour correspondences with colour neurons in V4 are more likely than others. A systematic search for such correlations has yet to be attempted and the few already undertaken have yielded ambiguous results (Day, 2001; Shanon, 1982). An analogy with the periodic table of elements might be appropriate. Initial attempts to classify elements produced a certain non-random clustering of properties (e.g. the alkaline metals vs the halogens) but no rhyme or reason could be discerned until Mendeleev noticed that when arranged according to atomic weights, the properties tended to repeat. When discrepancies emerged Mendeleev actually insisted that the empirical data on atomic weights was wrong, and later research has vindicated his view. Indeed he was even able to predict the existence — and properties — of missing elements. We believe it is only a matter of time before analogous correlations and patterns emerge for the rules of cross-activation in synaesthesia. For instance, it may not initially be obvious why the arbitrary vowel sequence AEIOU should map in a non-arbitrary manner to a certain sequence of colours (and we have seen some hints of this 50 V.S. RAMACHANDRAN AND E.M. HUBBARD

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Automaticity and localisation of concurrents predicts colour area activity in grapheme-colour synaesthesia

In grapheme-colour synaesthesia (GCS), the presentation of letters or numbers induces an additional 'concurrent' experience of colour. Early functional MRI (fMRI) investigations of GCS reported activation in colour-selective area V4 during the concurrent experience. However, others have failed to replicate this key finding. We reasoned that individual differences in synaesthetic phenomenology m...

متن کامل

A placebo-controlled investigation of synaesthesia-like experiences under LSD.

The induction of synaesthesia in non-synaesthetes has the potential to illuminate the mechanisms that contribute to the development of this condition and the shaping of its phenomenology. Previous research suggests that lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) reliably induces synaesthesia-like experiences in non-synaesthetes. However, these studies suffer from a number of methodological limitations in...

متن کامل

Insights from introspection: a commentary on Gould et al. (2014), “An extended case study on the phenomenology of spatial form synaesthesia”

Gould et al. (2014) refreshingly devote an entire paper to investigating the detailed experience of a synaesthete (AB), using a rigorous qualitative method. Their paper addresses sequence-space synaesthesia, in which people associate members of sequences such as months or numbers with specific spatial locations that together create an overall spatial pattern or “form” for each sequence. I have ...

متن کامل

Visual experiences in the blind induced by an auditory sensory substitution device.

In this report, the phenomenology of two blind users of a sensory substitution device - "The vOICe" - that converts visual images to auditory signals is described. The users both report detailed visual phenomenology that developed within months of immersive use and has continued to evolve over a period of years. This visual phenomenology, although triggered through use of The vOICe, is likely t...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2003